Review Article

 

Effects of perinatal undernutrition on juvenile play development of rats

Efectos de la desnutrición perinatal en el desarrollo del juego juvenil de ratas

 

1*Manuel Salas, 1Mirelta Regalado, 1Carmen Torrero

 

1Institute of Neurobiology, University of Mexico, Campus UNAM Juriquilla, Queretaro, Qro. Mexico.

 

Correspondencia: *Ph. D. Manuel Salas. Institute of Neurobiology. University of Mexico. Campus UNAM Juriquilla, Querétaro, Qro. México. E-mail: masal@unam.mx

 

DOI: https://doi.org/10.25009/eb.v14i34.2619

 

 

Recibido: Mayo 25, 2023   |   Aceptado: Julio 25, 2023

 

 

Resumen

En la rata el juego social es una respuesta innata, necesaria para promover el desarrollo de habilidades motrices, el rango social y el aprendizaje de respuestas esenciales para sobrevivir. Asimismo, el desarrollo del juego es un proceso complejo donde los protagonistas son expuestos a múltiples influencias sensoriales, y estresores ambientales incluyendo entre otros al aislamiento materno breve y el aporte de alimento. Tanto la malnutrición como la desnutrición perinatal afectan el desarrollo cerebral y sus funciones, al reducir el número de neuronas, depósito de mielina, número de dendritas y los contactos sinápticos en diversas estructuras corticales y subcorticales que participan en la integración del juego social. En esta revisión se analiza el impacto de diferentes modelos experimentales de restricción de alimento, y cómo el deficiente desarrollo sensorial asociado en diferentes áreas cerebrales pudiera impactar la transmisión, codificación de señales e integración de la actividad neuronal involucrada en el procesado del juego juvenil. Los hallazgos revisados pudieran ser relevantes para la comprensión de las alteraciones en el desarrollo del juego social, dependiente del trastorno sensorial causado por la restricción temprana de alimento. Estas alteraciones interfieren con mecanismos relacionados con la obtención de habilidades motoras, experiencia temprana y la cognición que pueden reconocerse durante la juventud y la adultez.

 

Palabras clave: Desnutrición perinatal, desarrollo sensorial y juego, ratas.

 

 

Abstract

In rats, juvenile play is an innate behavior that improves motor skills, builds social ranking, and provides basic learning experiences for survival. Juvenile play is also a complex process in which the animals are exposed to multisensory signals and environmental stressors, including maternal separation and perinatal food restriction. Perinatal malnutrition or undernutrition impedes brain growth and functions by decreasing neurogenesis, myelination, and the number of dendritic and synaptic contacts at different cortical and subcortical brain areas subserving juvenile play combinations. In this review we examined, in the rat, the impact of experimental models of perinatal food restriction, and how disordered sensory activities at different brain levels may disrupt the transmission, encoding and integration of the neuronal activity underlying the complex expression of juvenile play. Moreover, current findings may be relevant to understand how the sensory impairments provoked by perinatal undernutrition and its associated stressors may alter the mechanisms involved in motor skill development, early-life experiences and cognition as observed in youth and adulthood. 

 

Keywords: Early undernutrition, sensory and play development, rats.

 

 


1.  Introduction

In mammalian species the neuronal mechanisms underlying juvenile play and reproductive-like functions are integrated in the cortical and subcortical brain structures that evoke motivation, autonomic states, refined movement displacements and postural adaptive responses.1-4 Juvenile play is a sexually dimorphic, pleasurable, voluntary activity that consists in abundant stereotyped movements initiated when the animal is relaxed and healthy.5,6 Furthermore, juvenile play is a complex social response in which the participants are exposed to several multisensory environmental signals and conditions that may influence the organization and expression of late non-social behaviors like flight, hunting, fighting, and reproductive activities, among others.7,8 Unfortunately, evidence about the neuronal mechanisms underlying basic juvenile neuromotor activity in healthy and in handicapped and rehabilitated subjects is sometimes contradictory, as diverse non-physiological procedures, ages, and species have been used.9,10

In rodents, perinatal food restriction and the associated impoverishment of neonatal sensory stimulation are some of the non-genetic factors that may interfere with early brain development and are determinants for social behavioral disruption in adulthood.11,12 In the rat, perinatal undernutrition or malnutrition significantly interferes with brain development and functions by reducing neurogenesis, myelination, and the number and density of dendritic arbors, spines and synaptic contacts in different areas of the brain.13-19 These anatomical alterations associated with early sensory deficiencies result in an immature brain with impaired sensory and hormonal organization that affects an animal’s response to environmental demands.20 Furthermore, brain alterations may disturb the transmission, encoding and integration of ascending neuronal information from the peripheral receptors to the cerebral cortex, affecting the integration of reflex, locomotor and social motivational activities including juvenile play responses.2,21-30 The aim of this review was to analyze and compare the effects of different perinatal food restriction procedures, the associated impaired sensory maturation, and the role of mother-litter bond deficiencies in the mechanisms underlying the juvenile play development of rats.

 

 

2.   Undernutrition and juvenile play development

2.1. Effects of olfactory and cortical influences

Several reports showed that in the study of play fighting in rats, bilateral olfactory bulbectomy or induced anosmia by zinc-sulfate olfactory mucosa painting, at 23 or 24 days of age, did not affect the frequency of play fighting and play initiation by males, but slightly increased the frequency of play fighting without affecting play initiation in females. These findings suggested that early modulatory influences of the olfactory bulbs in the cortical and limbic structures and their association with amygdala activation were not especially relevant for play fighting expression.31 However, when total decorticated rats were assessed in like-lesioned pairs, the frequency of pinning—in which one rat stands over a supine partner during play fighting—was reduced about by 50%, and average pin durations were shorter. Furthermore, separate tests of play solicitation behaviors did not identify any differences between controls and decorticated subjects suggesting that play motivation remained intact. Although studies in neonatal decorticate rats indicate little effects on rough-and-tumble play, these may be explained by moderate impairments in motor-postural control and/or reduced cortical somatosensory excitability.32 Additionally, from postnatal days (PDs) 25 to 40, decorticate rats exhibited a 50% decrease in juvenile play fighting but increased the types of defensive responses compared to controls. The findings suggest that the cerebral cortex may interfere with the defensive responses of juvenile rats with prolonged ventral-ventral contact during play fighting.33 However, other studies have analyzed the effects of cortical and subcortical diffuse neuronal damages associated with neonatal undernutrition and sensory deprivation in the rat. The results showed that the total mean frequency of boxing, wrestling, and pinning markedly increased in male and female rats from PDs 20 to 60. Moreover, females exhibited an increase in juvenile play and during the interaction in pairs in the prepuberal period.34 These findings suggested that early food restriction and sensory deprivation were concurrent with reduced maternal anogenital licking and body contacts directed to the newborns which may have interfered with the somatosensory diffuse cortical and subcortical brain maturation, including the structures underlying juvenile play at peripheral and central modulatory mechanisms.19,27,30,31,35-37

In summary, the findings of these studies indicated that modulatory influences in the cortical and limbic structures via the olfactory bulbs and amygdala activation did not seem to be particularly relevant for juvenile play expression in rats. Furthermore, the findings showed that complete cerebral cortex removal decreases juvenile play fighting responses by 50% compared with sham-control subjects. Additionally, the diffuse cortical damages elicited by neonatal undernutrition also reduced play behavior in pairs or groups of rats, although food restriction and sensory stimulation increased the mean number of total dorsal body contacts, compared with pins directed to the nape and flank areas, which were significantly reduced (Fig.1).10 The effects of juvenile play on neonatal decorticate rats may be also explained by moderate impairments in motor cortical control mechanisms, as exhibited by increases in diverse types of defensive responses.32

The preweaning growth of young rats is highly dependent on intense mother/litter bonds that impact both maternal care activities and the newborn’s somatosensory and motor development including play performance. In this regard recent studies indicated that the maternal motivation of F1 underfed lactating dam’s changes throughout lactation. Thus, when maternal motivation was evaluated at PD 90, by recording deficiencies in retrieving, handling shavings, and crouching posture at PDs 4 and 12, the motivation of F1dams for pups was high (day 4) or declining at PD 12. Additionally, maternal motivation was correlated with c-Fos immunostaining of neurons in the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) and basolateral amygdala (BLA) when pups were removed from the dams 90 min after suckling. The findings showed that early underfed dams had Fos-I neuronal deficiencies in the mPFC, with minor effects on the BLA, possibly because the sensory cues required to evoke maternal motivation and possible the juvenile play motivation were suboptimal and/or due to deficient maternal network electrical transmission that are relevant to trigger deficient cognitive mother-litter bonds, mutually interfered with the neuronal network of adolescent rats for further play development.38 Current findings support the role of the maternal prefrontal cortex in the integration of sensory signals transmitted from different olfactory, visual, somatosensory, and auditory neuronal routes, with the limbic structures regulating the internal emotional states. Moreover, these sensory avenues are essential for cognitive spatiotemporal performance, such as maternal care, social play, attentiveness, and locomotor activity. Specifically, the mPFC, BLA and cerebellar vermis play an important role in maternal memory consolidation, decision making and conditioned learning.4,21,23,39-42 Studies in early underfed F1dams have evaluated the anatomical effects of reduction (Golgi-Cox) on the distal dendritic segments of layer III pyramidal neurons, perikaryon measurements, and the number of dendritic spines in the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), mPFC and BLA.38,43 In the newborn, the ACC, mPFC and BLA are a part of the neuronal networks associated with social play development, maternal response, motivation, and emotional states, among others.6 Unfortunately, current knowledge about how perinatal undernutrition influences juvenile play motivation is still limited. There is little information on the specific anatomical network damages underlying the motivational responses integrated at the brainstem level. Moreover, how they are possibly combined with the deficient multisensory arousal systems, and the origins of the autonomic nervous substrate associated with visceral activities and basic coordinated movements involved in juvenile play development.

 

 




Figure 1. Mean values ± of total body contacts directed at partners during the play in pairs or groups. Note the higher values in the dorsal area in the play in pairs and groups, with reductions in the LU and LUS groups, and minor values directed at other areas. Numbers above bars indicate different statistical comparisons (LC vs LU1, LCS vs LUS2, LC vs LCS3, LCS vs LU4, LU vs LUS5, LUS vs LC6).10

 


2.2. Effects of somatosensory influence

Maternal somatosensory stimulation is a relevant source of influence to modulate the expression of specific adaptive motor responses in young rats, including maternal anogenital licking, huddling, grooming, suckling, and retrieving. These responses are elicited by sensitive skin receptors located in strategic areas of the pup’s body. adaptive motor responses including reflex activity, locomotion, posture for retrieving, and juvenile play development, among others.5,10,22,25,44-49 In this context, a series of experiments have shown the relevant effects somatosensory stimulation of the skin to evoke juvenile play in rats. For example, local xylocaine anesthesia of the dorsal body surface of rats reduced the frequency of pinning during play performance, by 35% to 70%, while motivation to play remained intact. The findings suggested that dorsal body surface anesthetization decreased the stimulation of somatosensory receptors that trigger juvenile play expression. Thus, when untreated rats were paired with locally anesthetized rats, the untreated rats consistently pinned the dorsal body area of anesthetized rats more than vice versa but maintaining the motivational play response.25

Another point of interest to study the role of somatosensory influence was to analyze if during juvenile play in pairs or groups of subjects (n=4 per group) of the same sex, the somatosensory contacts given by F1 underfed rats at the nape, flanks, and the hairy dorsal skin areas to invite a partner to play, could have different effects on the frequency of hairy skin pinning throughout the testing sessions from PDs 15 to 60. According to several studies, pinning is a reliable indicator to measure juvenile play components in rats.6 Findings on the total number of pins given by an early F1 underfed rat to a partner showed that pins to the nape and flanks were unaffected or poorly activated, whereas pins to the hairy dorsal area decreased. Moreover, pinning increased more during juvenile play in pairs than in groups with perinatal undernutrition.10 Additionally, the effects of early handling for 5 min from PDs 1-7, and the exposure to a sensory-enriched environment for 30 min a day from PDs 8 to 30 to ameliorate the effects of the underfeeding maneuvers, were evaluated. The results indicated significant increments in the number of pins directed to the dorsal body area of the sham-ligated, underfed, sensory-stimulated controls (LCS) versus the nipple-ligated, and sensory stimulated (LUS) rats playing in pairs, with negligible effects on contacts directed to the other skin areas during group play (Fig. 1). Additionally, this peculiar profile of hairy skin contacts may be explained because early undernutrition reduces the pleasantness of tactile stimulation, which in turn interferes with juvenile play.50 The findings in LUS versus LCS group reductions may be related to a lower number of somatosensory receptors in dorsal skin or to low tactile skin excitability that diminishes the sense of pleasantness required to elicit juvenile play in underfed partners that did not receive relevant early sensory stimulation (Fig. 1).51 In line with this assumption, several studies have suggested that sensory deprivation associated with perinatal undernutrition may lead to an increase in high-frequency (60 kHz) ultrasonic calls and that sensory deprivation may cause an incomplete or distorted sensory image of cortical neuronal representation during ontogenetic development, thus affecting juvenile play performance.10,49,52,53 Furthermore, movement restriction decreases the number and distribution of spines along apical shafts of layer V pyramidal cells in the prefrontal and sensory cortices (rapid Golgi), the hippocampus and the basolateral amygdala (Golgi-Cox) in peripubertal rats, probably due to the stress from the restriction maneuver, which affects play expression.54,55 In summary the experimental findings showed that the dorsal skin area of early underfed rats may have a reduced number of somatosensory receptors and/or reduced excitability to evoke play behavior compared with other skin body regions. Furthermore, xylocaine anesthetization of the dorsal body surface of rats reduced the frequency of pinning by 35% to 70% with intact motivation to play compared with saline control subjects.

 

2.3. Thalamic relays and juvenile play behavior

Juvenile play in rats has also been evaluated by discrete electrolytic lesions in different key thalamic nuclei associated with the sensory modulation of ascending electrical signals reaching specific sensory regions of the cerebral cortex.25,56 Discrete lesions to the parafascicular or posterior thalamic regions significantly decrease the frequency of pinning in juvenile rats, while sparse lesions interfere with the motivation to play. Similar lesions placed within the ventrobasal thalamic nucleus have minimal effects on both pinning and play solicitation, whereas electrolytic lesions within ventrolateral aspects of the brain stem significantly reduce pinning, with no effects on play motivation. These findings are inconclusive because the behavioral experiments used to evaluate the potential damage mechanisms may be more anatomically and functionally complicated. However, the overall pattern of results indicates that these thalamic areas may be involved in the sensory modulatory mechanisms underlying juvenile play.25 Additionally, similar studies showed that rough-and tumble social play, evaluated by the frequency of pinning, was significantly reduced in the dorsomedial thalamus and parafascicular region of juvenile rats by 33% and 73%, respectively. Moreover, the parafascicular lesions elicited effects on average pin durations, making them 105% longer than those of controls; and lesions in the parafascicular but not in the dorsomedial regions reduced play solicitation behaviors.57 These results of discrete thalamic lesions and juvenile play development indicated that the changes were limited because the site, extension of lesions, and lack of basic neuronal excitatory/inhibitory thalamic connectivity were not well-known.

Additionally, several studies have suggested that the thalamic reticular nucleus (TRN) and the lateral thalamic nucleus (LTN) are also associated with the control of sensory afferent transmission.58 In this regard, the use of neonatal undernutrition in rats by transferring half of the litter (n=4) from the nest to an incubator for 12 h a day from PDs 1 to 23 resulted in significant reductions of the cell body and dendritic field areas (Golgi-Cox), as well as in the number of dendritic branches of TRN neurons, with no significant differences between groups when similar neuronal measurements in the LTN were evaluated.56 In summary these findings suggest that discrete thalamic electrolytic lesions may result in variable deficiencies in juvenile play performance. Furthermore, they provide additional information of neuronal alterations in some of the thalamic nuclei associated with sensory transmission in early underfed rats. These morphofunctional alterations may disrupt the sensory ascending modulatory mechanisms underlying social play development.1,10 However, more experimental data on the modulatory thalamic sensory relays under different developmental conditions are needed to understand the role of ascending/descending neuronal discharges in triggering autonomic and motor-powered juvenile play expression in rats.

 

 

2.4. Effects of auditory stimulation on juvenile performance

A study of juvenile play development in rats with early auditory deficiencies showed that a punctured tympanic bilateral membrane with the pinna of the ear folded over the ear opening significantly reduced juvenile play.25 In other experiments, an analysis of high-frequency 50-kHz ultrasonic vocalizations (USVs) emitted by pairs of control rats before and during juvenile play revealed that auditory interactions were significantly abundant and associated with a confident affective state eliciting communicative playful body contacts with the partner.59 Furthermore, when both partners in a playing pair were devocalized, the frequency of playful attacks was significantly disrupted. When play was assessed between a vocal and a non-vocal partner, rats preferred to play with the one able to vocalize. By contrast, in a similar study 50 kHz calls by juvenile rats did not signal playful interactions compared with adult rats.59 The findings suggest that in juvenile rats, 50 kHz USVs play a small role in maintaining a playful mood and generating auditory signals to one another during play fighting. In general, the authors conclude that vocalizations have a slight role in juvenile play but serve a more significant role in modulating adult interactions between strangers, allowing for the tactical mitigation of the risk of aggression.59 Additionally, malnourished male rats fed with a deficient diet (6% casein) before mating and during pregnancy and tested for USVs after brief nest isolation or cooling conditions at PDs 7, 9 and 11, were compared with well-nourished pups (25% casein). Thus, prenatally malnourished pups emitted a smaller variety of calls, with fewer ascending USVs at PDs 7 and 9, and greater descending USVs at PD 11. The findings reflect altered interactions in central brain structures and functional maturation that may predict deficits in adaptive behavior including juvenile play at later ages.60

According to previous morphological studies on neonatal undernourishment in rats, in which half of the litter (4 out of 8 pups) were removed from the nest and placed in an incubator (12 h) from PDs 1-23, Golgi-Cox-stained bipolar neurons in the medial superior olive presented alterations in the dendritic arbor and binaural modulatory interactions with the auditory axon terminals.61 These findings may anticipate that the modulatory system at the first relays of the auditory pathway may disrupt the central integration of the afferent auditory signals of play involved in the binaural interactions between the axon terminals, and the ipsilateral and contralateral dendritic arbors probably interfere play expression. Unfortunately, this underfeeding paradigm has not been used to evaluate its effects on play development in the juvenile rat. In summary, the 50 kHz USVs emitted by pairs of well-fed rats before and during juvenile playful interactions were significantly abundant and associated with certain affective states with the partner. Furthermore, prenatally malnourished pups emitted a smaller variety of calls, with fewer ascending USVs at PDs 7 and 9, and greater descending USVs at PD 11. The findings may indicate altered interactions in central brain structures, and functional disorders that may result in long-term brain deficits that affect juvenile play expression.

 

2.5. The hippocampus and juvenile play development

The hippocampus is a brain structure necessary for spatial learning, memory and attentive responses that requires relevant environmental sensory information during play activity, particularly somatosensory, visual, and auditory signals.62,63 Moreover, the hippocampus is overly sensitive to the noxious influence of different stressors such as early food restriction and sensory deprivation.19 Thus, prenatally and neonatally undernourished rats tested as adults in an 8-arm and then a 16-arm radial maze showed significantly more error distributions in the complex maze and in the time taken to make the choices in the spatial memory task compared with the well-fed controls.64 These behavioral deficiencies were correlated with morphometric alterations (according to rapid Golgi) in the hippocampal growth of 2-month-old rats fed with a low protein diet (8% casein) for 6, 12 and 18 months, compared with age-matched control rats and recovered rats, which were nourished first with a low protein diet (8% casein) and then with a normal diet (25% casein) for 6 months. The total number of granular hilar neurons of CA1 and CA3 pyramidal cells was significantly decreased in malnourished adult rats, including the food recovered group not tested during the juvenile stage. Additionally, early undernourished adult subjects exhibited impaired high-frequency electrical stimulation of hippocampal dentate granule cells to produce long-term potentiation and visuospatial learning compared with their controls, suggesting a clear dysfunction associated with early malnutrition in adult hippocampal behavioral activities, perhaps related to juvenile play performance.39,65,66 However, the role of hippocampal system deficits caused by perinatal undernutrition in juvenile play development requires further investigation, particularly to understand the neuronal circuits involved in play and the long-term consequences of these deficits on cognition in healthy subject and subjects with brain disorders.67 In line with the above, studies on the neurophysiological interactions between the prefrontal cortex and hippocampus have determined that when the correlation or synchronicity of these interactions is disrupted, they cause alterations in cognition, emotions, fear and anxiety like in psychiatric disease.68 Additionally, these studies are consistent with experimental evidence that, in the parietal association cortex, thalamo-cortical projections for recruiting and spindling-like responses, elicited by repetitive (6-9 sec) stimulation of the centrum medianum-parafascicular complex, the intralaminar thalamic nuclei that are synchronized and mutually connected for predictive behavioral responses.69 These cortical and subcortical structures are a part of the complex neuronal networks involved in the expression of social play in rats. Unfortunately, the functional interactions between these brain structures associated with early food restriction during the play routine have not been clearly elucidated.

 

2.6. Effects of visual stimulation

Several studies on play fighting behavior of blinded or sighted juvenile rats used the brief paired-encounter procedure, in which rats were observed in large or small chambers on alternate days from PDs 28-43. The findings showed no significant differences in play behavior regardless of pair composition or chamber size.70 The authors conclude that vision is not relevant for initiating or maintaining play fighting. Play behavior in rats initiates after the opening of the ears and eyes around PD 18, reaches a peak at PD 20, and then gradually declines until PDs 30-40, remaining stable until adulthood.71 This behavioral sequence is concurrent with the development of the morphological visual pathway and the maturation of electrophysiological evoked cortical responses.21,72 In this context, a possible explanation for the lack of effects of vision on juvenile play performance may be that the researchers evaluated play behavior at a late stage (PDs 28-43), when play activity was already reduced and stabler.6,73 In a similar study the effects of optic enucleation on play fighting were evaluated in infant golden hamsters from PDs 30-36. The authors noted that the differences in pinning, play fighting and grooming time, as well as in the percentage of time spent in play fighting and the amount of locomotion between blind-blind, blind-intact, and intact-intact dyads were small and statistically insignificant.74 It is possible that in other species the lack of vision may be compensated by sensory systems that are more closely related to the physical contact stimulation that play performance requires. Additionally, juvenile rats living under dim light conditions and tested for play behavior when exposed to an intensely lit arena showed suppressed pinning and decreased boxing/wrestling social responses. The findings indicate that social responses in adolescent rats may be disrupted by distinct levels of environmental organization or light conditions, even when the visual system is not damaged.7 In summary, the role of early undernutrition and its interaction with sight impairments during juvenile play is still poorly understood.

 

 

3.   Mother-litter bond influences

In mammals, mother-litter interactions play a fundamental role in species preservation by eliciting plastic brain changes in the mother to produce maternal behavior adaptive mechanisms directed to the newborn for their survival and  brain and behavioral development.19,75 Several studies have shown that perinatal undernutrition causes lasting deficits in maternal care, including reduced nest building, nursing time, retrieval responses, licking of pups, and number of contacts with pups, compared to controls.11 In this context, rat pups emit different USVs that may serve as a powerful means of communication with the mother to promote their physiological development. These vocalizations have been studied under several distressing and motivational situations, such as separation from the dam (frequency range from 30 to 50 kHz) and exposure of the newborns to cold, acute isolation, and pain cues at 22 kHz.76-78 Furthermore, USVs for communication may be altered by perinatal malnutrition (6% casein) and associated damage to various immature brain structures when compared with well-nourished controls (25 % casein).60 From other studies, it is known that brain stem areas associated with swallowing, breathing, and laryngeal motor innervations are also necessary for phonation.79,80 Neuroanatomical tracing studies provide a description of the central brain stem connections of the axons within the superior and recurrent laryngeal nerves, the latter with a special motor innervation to the intrinsic laryngeal muscles arising from the caudal ambiguous nucleus (AMBc) motoneurons.81,82 These muscles and the vocal cord morphology change in neonatally underfed rat pups.83 A pioneer study of USV recordings in malnourished rats indicated that pup USVs reduced the dam’s nursing motivation in association with reduced olfactory, visual, and auditory stimuli to localize, retrieve and protect the young in a safe nest environment.60 In a complementary Golgi-Cox morphometric study, our group evaluated the impact of perinatal undernutrition and the salutary effects of daily body massage stimulation (10 min) from PDs 4-15 on AMBc multipolar motoneuron development.84 These motoneurons were related to the laryngeal intrinsic muscle innervation to produce USVs in neonatal rats.82 The results indicated that multipolar motoneuron dendritic scores were reduced in both number and density at PDs 8, 12 and 15 in the underfed subjects. Moreover, the somatic massage stimulation of pups for a daily 10 min span (PDs 4 to 15) increased the body weight and the values of morphological parameters in both experimental groups, although the dendritic arbors of underfed and massage-stimulated rats only reduced their values at PD 15.84 In summary, these experimental findings showed that USV alterations associated with perinatal malnutrition or early undernutrition disrupted the mother-litter bonds. Moreover, the USV alterations were associated with neuronal damage in various brain structures underlying the integration of neonatal vocal communication at critical stages of life. Additionally, the 50 kHz USV descending pattern of perinatally underfed rats was reduced and not modified by early massage stimulation.84 The current findings indicate that perinatal food restriction disrupts the sensory morpho-functional brain organization subserving phonation and affects the USV calls emitted during juvenile play expression of rats, in association with other concurrent environmental factors during lactation and prepuberal stages. In this regard, our findings are in line with recent studies on the memory processes and anxiety-like behavior severely interfered when early undernutrition was induced by the large litter sizes exposure.85

 

 

4. Perinatal stress and juvenile play development

Another point of interest concerns to the role of the perinatal underfeeding paradigm as a stressor agent affecting the anatomical and behavioral mother-infant interactions by increasing the release of glucocorticoids, which reprogram the Hypothalamic-Pituitary-Adrenal (HPA) axis of  the newborn to present non-adaptive juvenile play responses.86 In this regard, it is known that during the first week of a rat’s life there is a period of low or no stress response to hypothermia, electric shock, or brief periods of maternal separation. This depends on the immaturity of the portal blood vessels arising from the hypothalamic median eminence or the maturation of distinct cell types in the anterior pituitary gland of rats.87,88 In a recent study of play development in the rat the underfeeding paradigm was initiated in F0 dams during gestation by 50% to 70% of food restriction.10 which affected the placental weight and functions causing deficiencies in fetal nutrition and growth as previously described.89 Additionally, from PDs 1 to 24, F1 underfed pups spent 12 h with a nipple-ligated dam and 12 h with a normally lactating mother, where they received maternal sensory stimulation in the encounter that ameliorated their low body weight and/or reduced food consumption.90 Thereafter, UG F1 virgins slightly increased food consumption and body weight through a balanced diet from PDs 25 to 90, without compensating the low body weight, which was associated with their juvenile play deficiencies and disrupted bonding interactions with the foster pups.91 Although the effects of brief neonatal separation from the mother on early development were attenuated in this study, the effects of gestational stress were not prevented. Thus, a prenatal stress component may affect the connections of pyramidal and multipolar neurons within the juvenile play networks, increasing the glucocorticoid levels that reprogram the HPA axis of the early underfed young to provide non-adaptive social behavioral responses.92 This assumption may be supported by the long-term deficiencies associated with early undernutrition and social stressors in juvenile play response, disturbed social interactions, increased responses to novelty, deficient learning, decreased exploratory behavior and brain disorders in adulthood.67,93 The current data suggest that early food restriction results in long-term impoverished maternal responses that may correlate with different hypoplastic neuronal effects on the juvenile play relays, which may significantly and differentially affect the thresholds and propagation of action potentials, disrupting the synaptic plasticity underlying early cognitive responses such as juvenile play expression. In summary of the neuronal interactions occurring during the juvenile play expression in rats is grossly depicted in Figure 2.

 




Figure 2. Schematic neuronal cortical/subcortical relay interactions occurring during the juvenile play expression of rats. Asterisks, indicate places where early food restriction interfered the neuronal play development. See text for abbreviations. Data partly taken from.38

 

 


5. Conclusions

The aim of this review was to analyze and compare the effects of different procedures of perinatal food restriction in rats and the associated sensory maturation impairment and mother-litter bond deficiencies on the mechanisms underlying juvenile play performance. In summary, deficits in the maturation of the olfactory, gustatory, visual, somatosensory, and auditory systems of newborns, commonly seen in early food restriction protocols, severely disrupt brain development and functions. Moreover, repeated separation of pups from the dams as an early stressor interferes with the sensory communication required to achieve early experience and develop motor abilities for the juvenile play expression of the progeny. Additionally, deficient mother-litter bonds early in life may also affect the pup’s brain growth, behavioral repertoire, and adaptive stress responses, all of which have been associated with mental disorders and the propensity for addictions at later ages. Furthermore, because the neuronal mechanisms underlying social play and maternal-like responses varies with the activation of ancient subcortical and cortical structures eliciting autonomic and motivational states, the study of social play may be relevant to modulate motoric skill abilities and defensive environmental adaptive responses. Most of our knowledge on synaptic networks deficiencies subserving social play in mammal species, emerged from peripheral and central lesions associated to early malnutrition or perinatal food restriction. Thus, morphometric rapid and Golgi-Cox staining  neuronal deficiencies and the reduced Fos-I activation of cortical and subcortical structures were associated with lower motivation and neuronal communication associated with early food restriction  Finally, under these conditions, the juvenile play development model in the rat may be a useful tool to analyze, during a sensitive stage in life, the impacts of a series of noxious developmental factors disrupting the mechanisms for gathering early experience and cognition.

 

 

6. Conflict of interest

The authors declare that there is no conflict of interest regarding the publication of this article.

 

 

7. Acknowledgements

Partly supported by DGAPA/UNAM, IN200317. We thank Jessica González-Norris for editorial assistance.

 

 

8.   Referencias

 

1.     Groenewegen HJ, Uylings HBM. The prefrontal cortex and the integration of sensory limbic and autonomic information. Progr Brain Res. 2000 126: 3–28.

 

2.     Numan M. Motivational systems and the neural circuitry of maternal behavior in the rat. Dev Psychobiol, 2007 29: 12–21.

 

3.     Held SDE, Špinka M. Animal play and animal welfare. Anim Behav. 2011 81: 891–899.

 

4.     Van Kerkhof LM, Damsteegt R, Treza, V, Voor P, Vanderschuren JMJ. Social play behavior in adolescent rats is mediated by functional activity in medial prefrontal cortex and striatum. Neuropsychophar. 2013 38: 1899-1909.

 

5.     Auger AP, Olesen KM. Brain sex differences and the organization of juvenile social play behavior. J Neuroendocrinol. 2009 21: 519-525.

 

6.     Panksepp J, Biven L. Playful dreamlike circuits of the brain. The ancestral sources of social joy and laughter. In: J Panksepp. and Biven L. (Eds.). The archaeology of mind. WW Norton & Company, New York. 2012, 351-387.

7.     Vanderschuren LJ, Niesink BM, Spruijt BM, Van Ree JM. Influence of environmental factors on social play behavior of juvenile rats. Physiol Behav. 1995 58: 119-123.

 

8.     Klein ZA, Padow VA, Romeo RO. The effects of stress on play and home cage behaviors in adolescent male rats. Dev Psychobiol. 2009 52: 62-70.

 

9.     Panksepp J, Burgdorf J. “Laughing” rats and the evolutionary antecedents of human joy? Physiol Behav, 2003 79: 533–547.

 

10.  Salas M, Regalado M, Torrero C, Ortiz-Valladares M. Juvenile play behavior in neonatally underfed and sensory stimulated Wistar rats. J Behav Brain Sci. 2019 9: 362-374.

 

11.  Salas M, Torrero C, Pulido S. Long-term alterations in the maternal behavior of neonatally undernourished rats. Physiol Behav. 1984 33: 273-278.

 

12.  Salas M, Regalado M, Torrero, C. Recovery of long-term maternal behavioral deficiencies of neonatally underfed rats by early sensory stimulation: Effects of successive parturitions. Nutr Neurosci. 2001 4: 311–322.

 

13.  Salas M, Diaz S, Nieto A. Effects of neonatal food deprivation on cortical spines and dendritic development of the rat. Brain Res. 1974 73: 139-144.

 

14.  Krigman MR, Hogan EL. Undernutrition in the developing rat: effect upon myelination. Brain Res. 1976 107: 239–255.

 

15.  Morgan BLG, Naismith DJ. The effect of early postnatal undernutrition on the growth and development of the rat brain. Brit J Nutr. 1982 48: 15-23.

 

16.  Pascual R, Zamora-León SP. Effects of neonatal maternal deprivation and postweaning environmental complexity on dendritic morphology of prefrontal pyramidal neurons in the rat. Acta Neurobiol Exper. 2007 67: 471–479.

 

17.  Breton C, Lukaszewski MA, Risold PY, Enache M, Guillemot J, Rivière G, Delahaye F, Lesage J, Dutriez-Casteloot I, Laborie Ch, Vieau D. Maternal prenatal undernutrition alters the response of POMC neurons to energy status variation in adult male rat offspring. Am J Physiol Endocrinol Metab. 2009 296: 462–472.

 

18.  Torrero C, Regalado M, Rubio L, Salas M. Effects of neonatal undernutrition on development of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex pyramidal cells in the rat. J Behav Brain Sci. 2014 4: 49–57.

 

19.  Ortiz-Valladares M, Torrero C, Regalado M, Salas M. Late-emerging effects of perinatal undernutrition in neuronal limbic structures underlying the maternal response in the rat. Brain Res, 2018 1700: 31–40.

 

20.  Smith SG, Northcutt KV. Perinatal hypothyroidism increases play behaviors in juvenile rats. Horm Behav. 2018 98: 1-7.

 

21.  Callison DA, Spencer JW. Effect of chronic undernutrition and/or visual deprivation upon the visual evoked potential from the developing rat brain. Dev Psychobiol. 1968 1: 196–204.

 

22.  Altman J, Sudarshan K, Das GD, McCormick N, Barnes R. The influence of nutrition on neural and behavioral development. III. Development of some motor, particularly locomotor patterns during infancy. Dev Psychobiol. 1970 4: 97-114.

 

23.  Salas M, Cintra L. Nutritional influences upon somatosensory evoked responses during development in the rat. Physiol Behav. 1973 10: 1019-1022.

 

24.  Math F. Davrainville JL. Electrophysiological study on the postnatal development of mitral cell activity in the rat olfactory bulb. Influence of undernutrition. Brain Res. 1980 194: 223-227.

 

25.  Siviy SM, Panksepp J. Sensory modulation of juvenile play in rats. Dev Psychobiol. 1987 20: 39-55.

 

26.  Clarke KA, Parker AJ, Smart JL. Analysis of walking locomotion in adult female rats undernourished as suckling. Physiol Behav. 1992 52: 823-826.

 

27.  Frias C, Torrero, C, Regalado M, Salas M. Development of mitral cells and olfactory bulb layers in neonatally undernourished rats. Nutr Neurosci. 2009 12: 96-103.

 

28.  Harmon KM, Cromwell HC, Burgdorf J, Moskal JR, Brudzynski SM, Kroes RA, Panksepp, J. Rats selectively bred for low levels of 16 kHz ultrasonic vocalizations exhibit alterations in early social motivation. Dev Psychobiol. 2008 50: 322–331.

 

29.  Pellis SM, Pellis VC, Bell HC. The function of play in the development of social brain. American Journal of play. 2010 2: 278-296.

 

30.  Northcutt KV, Leal-Medina TS, Yoon YS. Early postnatal hypothyroidism reduces juvenile play behavior, but prenatal hypothyroidism compensates for these effects. Physiol Behav. 2021 241: 113594.1980.

 

31.  Beatty WW, Costello KB. Olfactory bulbectomy and play fighting in juvenile rats. Physiol Behav. 1983 30: 525–528.

 

32.  Panksepp J, Normansell L, Cox JF, Siviy SM. Effects of neonatal decortication on the social play of juvenile rats. Physiol Behav. 1994 56: 429-443.

 

33.  Pellis SM, Pellis VC, Whishaw IQ. The role of the cortex in play fighting by rats: developmental and evolutionary implications. Brain Behav Evol. 1992 39: 270-284.

 

34.  Loranca A, Torrero C, Salas M. Development of play behavior in neonatally undernourished rats. Physiol Behav. 1999 66: 3-10.

 

35.  Birke LI, Sadler D. Differences in maternal behavior of rats and the sociosexual development of the offspring. Dev Psychobiol. 1987 20: 85-89.

 

36.  Álaez C, Calvo R, Obregon MJ, Pascual-Leone AM. Thyroid hormones and 5´-deiodinase activity in neonatal undernourished rats. Endocrinology. 1992 130: 773-779.

 

37.  Escobar C, Salas M. Neonatal undernutrition and amygdaloid nuclear complex development: an experimental study in the rat. Exper Neurol. 1993 122: 311-318.

 

38.  Salas M, Ortiz-Valladares M, Regalado M, Torrero C. Neuronal c-Fos reactivity of medial prefrontal cortex and basolateral amygdala in perinatally underfed lactating Wistar rats. J Behav Brain Sci. 2020 10: 548-561.

 

39.  Tonkiss J, Galler JR, Shukitt-Hale B, Rocco FJ. Prenatal protein malnutrition impairs visual discrimination learning in adult rats. Psychobiology. 1991 19: 247-250.

 

40.  Chavez CM, McGaugh JL, Weinberger NM. Activation of the basolateral amygdala induces long-term enhancement of specific memory representations in the cerebral cortex. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2013 101: 8-18.

 

41.  Hernandez-Briones ZS, Garcia-Bañuelos P, Lopez ML, Martinez ACh, Carrillo P, Coria-Avila G, Manzo J, García LI. Olfactory stimulation induces cerebellar vermis activation during sexual learning in male rats. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2017 146: 31-36.

 

42.  Rushmore RJ, McGaughy JA, Amaral AC, Mokler DJ, Morgane PJ, Galler JR, Rosene DL. The neural basis of attentionally alterations in prenatally protein malnourished rats. Cerebral Cortex. 2021 31: 497-512.

 

43.  Salas M, Torrero C, Regalado M, Rubio L. Effects of perinatal undernutrition on the basilar dendritic arbor of the anterior cingulate pyramidal neurons in lactating dams. Acta Neurobiol Exp (Wars). 2015 75: 372-380.

 

44.  Brewster J, Leon M. Facilitation of maternal transport by Norway rat pups. J Comp Physiol Psychol. 1980 94: 80–88.

 

45.  Stern JM, Johnson SK. Ventral somatosensory determinants of nursing behavior in Norway rats. I. Effects of variations in the quality and quantity of pup stimuli. Physiol Behav. 1990 47: 993-1011.

 

46.  Moore CL, Power KL. Variations in maternal care and individual differences in play, exploration, and grooming of juvenile Norway rat offspring. Dev Psychobiol. 1992 25: 165-182.

 

47.  Brouette–Lahlou I, Godinot F, Vernet–Maury E. The mother rat’s vomeronasal organ is involved in detection of dodecyl propionate, the pup’s preputial gland pheromone. Physiol Behav. 1999 66: 427–436.

 

48.  Soriano O, Regalado M, Torrero C, Salas M. Contributions of undernutrition and handling to huddling development of rats. Physiol Behav. 2006 89: 543-551.

 

49.  Felix J, Regalado M, Torrero C, Salas, M. Retrieval of pups by female rats undernourished during the pre- and neonatal period. J Behav Brain Sci. 2014 4: 325-333.

 

50.  Olausson H, Wessberg J, Morrison I, McGlone F, Vallbo A. The neurophysiology of unmyelinated tactile afferents. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2010 34: 185-191.

 

51.  Ackerley R, Carlsson I, Wester H, Olausson H, Backlund Wasling HB. Touch perception across skin sites: differences between sensitivity, direction discrimination and pleasantness. Front Behav Neurosci. 2014 8: 54.

 

52.  Xerri C, Coq JO, Merzenich MM, Jenkins WM. Experience-induced plasticity of cutaneous maps in the primary somatosensory cortex of adult monkeys and rats. J Physiol (Paris). 1996 90: 277-287.

 

53.  Kaidbey JH, Ranger M, Myers MM, Anwar M, Ludwig RJ, Schulz AM, Barone JI, Kolacz J, Welch MG. Early life maternal separation and maternal behaviour modulate acoustic characteristics of rat pup ultrasonic vocalizations. Sci Rep. 2019 9: 19012.

 

54.  Sala-Catala J, Torrero C, Regalado M, Salas M, Ruiz-Marcos A. Movements restriction and alterations of the number of spines distributed along the apical shafts of layer V pyramids in motor and primary sensory cortices of the peripubertal and adult rat. Neuroscience. 2005 133: 137-145.

 

55.  Pinzón-Parra CA, Vidal-Jiménez B, Camacho-Abrego I, Flores-Gómez AA, Rodríguez-Moreno A, Flores G. Juvenile stress causes reduced locomotor behavior and dendritic spine density in the prefrontal cortex and basolateral amygdala in Sprague-Dawley rats. Synapse, 2018 73: Iss, 1/ e22066.

 

56.  Salas M, Torrero C, Pulido S. Undernutrition induced by early pup separation delays the development of the thalamic reticular nucleus in rats. Exper Neurol. 1986 93: 447-455.

 

57.  Siviy SM, Panksepp J. Dorsomedial diencephalic involvement in the juvenile play of rats. Behav Neurosci. 1985 99: 1103-1113.

 

58.  Yingling CD, Skinner JE. Selective regulation of thalamic sensory relay nucleus by nucleus reticularis thalami. Electroenceph Clin Neurophysiol. 1976 41: 476-482.

 

59.  Kisko TM, Euston DR, Pellis SM. Are 50-khz calls used as play signals in the playful interactions of rats? The effects of devocalization on play with unfamiliar partners as juveniles and as adults. Behav Proc. 2015 113: 113-121.

 

60.  Tonkiss J, Bonnie KE, Hudson JL, Shultz PL, Duran P, Galler JR. Ultrasonic call characteristics of rat pups are altered following prenatal malnutrition. Dev Psychobiol. 2003 43: 90-101.

 

61.  Salas M, Torrero C, Regalado M, Martinez-Gomez M, Pacheco P. Dendritic arbor alterations in the medial superior olivary neurons of neonatally underfed rats. Acta Anat. 1994 151: 180-187.

 

62.  Howland, JG., and Wang, YT. Synaptic plasticity in learning and memory: stress effects in the hippocampus. Progr Brain Res. 2008 169: 145-158.

 

63.  Xiong H, Krugers HJ. Tuning hippocampal synapses by stress-hormones relevance for emotional memory formation. Brain Res. 2015 1621: 114-120.

 

64.  Jordan TC, Cane SE, Howells KF. Deficits in spatial memory performance induced by early undernutrition. Dev Psychobiol. 1981 14: 317-325.

 

65.  Jordan TC, Clark GA. Early undernutrition impairs hippocampal long-term potentiation in adult rats. Behav Neurosci. 1983 97: 319-322.

 

66.  Diaz-Cintra S, Cintra L, Galvan A, Aguilar A, Kemper T, Morgane PJ. Effects of prenatal protein deprivation on postnatal development of granule cells in the fascia dentata. J Comp Neurol. 1991 310: 356-364.

 

67.  Andersen SL. Stress, sensitive periods, and substance abuse. Neurobiol Stress. 2019 10: 100140.

 

68.  Sigurdsson T, Duvarci, S. Hippocampal-prefrontal interactions in cognition, behavior, and psychiatric disease. Front Syst Neurosci. 2016 9: 190.

 

69.  Sasaki K, Matsuda Y, Mizuna N. Thalamocortical projections for recruiting responses and spindling-like responses in the parietal cortex. Exp Brain Res. 1975 22: 87-96.

 

70.  Bierley RA, Huges SL, Beatty WW. Blindness and play fighting in juvenile rats. Physiol Behav. 1986 36: 199-201.

 

71.  Thor DH, Holloway WRJr. Developmental analyses of social play behavior in juvenile rats. Bull Psychonom Soc. 1984 22: 587-590.

 

72.  Salas M, Schapiro S. Hormonal influences upon the maturation of the rat brain´s responsiveness to sensory stimuli. Physiol Behav. 1970 5: 7-11.

 

73.  Panksepp J. The ontogeny of play in rats. Dev Psychobiol. 1981 14: 327-332.

 

74.  Guerra RG, Vieira ML, Gasparetto S, Takase E. Effects of blindness on play fighting in Golden hamster infants. Physiol Behav. 1989 46: 775-777.

 

75.  Salcedo C, Torrero C, Regalado M, Rubio L, Salas M. Effects of pre- and neonatal undernutrition on the kyphotic response and c-Fos activity in the caudal periaqueductal gray of primiparous lactating Wistar rats. Physiol Behav. 2018 185: 87-94.

 

76.  Brudzynski P, Kehoe P, Callahan M. Sonographic structure of isolation-induced ultrasonic calls of rat pups. Dev Psychobiol. 1999 34: 195-204.

 

77.  D´Amato FR, Scalera E, Sarli C, Moles A. Pups call, mothers rush: does maternal responsiveness affects the amount of ultrasonic vocalizations in mouse pups? Behav Genet. 2005 35: 103-112.

 

78.  Harmon KM, Cromwell HC, Burgdorf J, Moskal JR, Brudzynski SM, Kroes RA, Panksepp, J. Rats selectively bred for low levels of 50 kHz ultrasonic vocalizations exhibit alterations in early social motivation. Dev Psychobiol. 2008 50: 322-331.

 

79.  Sawcezuk A, Mosier KM. Neural control of tongue movement with respect to respiration and swallowing. Curr Rev Oral Biol Med. 2001 12: 18-37.

 

80.  Branchi I, Santucci D, Alleva E. Analysis of ultrasonic vocalizations emitted by infant rodents. Curr Protocol Toxicol. 2006 30: 1-14.

 

81.  Patrickson JW, Smith TE, Zhou SS. Afferent projections of the superior and recurrent laryngeal nerves. Brain Res. 1991 539: 169-174.

 

82.  Riede T. Subglotal pressure, tracheal airflow, and intrinsic laryngeal muscle activity during rat ultrasound vocalization. J Neurophysiol. 2011 106: 2580-2592.

 

83.  Pascual-Font A, Hernandez-Morato I, McHanwell S, Vazquez T, Maranillo E, Sañudo J, Valderrama-Canales FJ. The central projections of the laryngeal nerves in the rat. J Anat. 2011 219: 217-228.

 

84.  Lopez-Jimenez D, Torrero C, Regalado M, Salas M. Effects of perinatal undernutrition and massage stimulation upon the ambiguous nucleus in the rat prior to weaning. J Behav Brain Sci. 2013 3: 200-209.

 

85.  Dorantes-Barrios CJ, Domínguez-Salazar E, González-Flores O, Cortés-Barberena E, Hurtado-Alvarado G. Behavioral consequences of postnatal undernutrition and enriched environment during later life. Physiol Behav. 2021 241: 113566.

 

86.  Lesage J, Sebaai N, Leonhardt M, Dutriez-Casteloot I, Breton C, Deloof S, Vieau D. Perinatal maternal undernutrition programs the offspring hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis. Stress, 2006 9: 183-198.

 

87.  Schapiro S. Maturation of the neuroendocrine response to stress in the rat. Chapter VIII. Early experience and behavior. The psychobiology of development. (Eds. G Newton & S Levine). Charles C Thomas publisher. 1968 Pp. 198-257.

 

88.  Weinstock M. Alterations induced by gestational stress in brain morphology and behaviour of the offspring. Progr Neurobiol. 2001 65: 427-451.

 

89.  Belkacemi L, Nelson DM, Desai M, Ross MG. Maternal undernutrition influences placental-fetal development. Biol Reprod. 2010 83: 325-331.

 

90.  Lee MHS, Williams DI. Changes in licking behaviour of rat mother following handling of young, Anim Behav. 1974 22: 679-681.

 

91.  Sefcikova Z, Mozes S. Effect of early nutritional experience on the feeding bahaviour of adult female rats. Vet Med (Praha). 2002 47: 315-322.

 

92.  Murmu MS, Salomon S, Biala M, Weinstock K, Braun K, Bock J. Changes of spine density and dendritic complexity in the prefrontal cortex in offspring of mothers exposed to stress during pregnancy. Eur J Neurosci. 2006 24: 1447-1487.

 

93.  Cordoba NE, Arolfo MP, Brioni JD, Orsingher OA. Perinatal undernutrition impairs spatial learning in recovered adult rats. Acta Physiol Pharmacol Ther Latinoam. 1994 44: 70-76.

Enlaces refback

  • No hay ningún enlace refback.


 

eNeurobiología es una publicación cuatrimestral editada por el Instituto de Investigaciones Cerebrales de la Universidad Veracruzana. Estamos ubicados en Av. Dr. Luis Castelazo Ayala, s/n, colonia Industrial Ánimas, C.P. 91190, Xalapa-Enríquez, Veracruz, México. Teléfono: 8418900 ext. 13062, www.iice.uv.mx; eneurobiologia@uv.mx. Reserva de Derechos al Uso Exclusivo 04-2023-061314100600-102, otorgada por el Instituto Nacional de Derechos de Autor. ISSN: 2007-3054. Esta obra está bajo una Licencia Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International.