Research article
Maternal behavioral disorders associated
with neuronal changes in the lateral habenula of pre- and neonatally underfed
rats
Conducta
maternal alterada asociada a cambios neuronales en la habénula lateral de ratas
con desnutrición pre- y neonatal
1*Manuel Salas, 1Carmen Torrero, 1Mirelta
Regalado
1Department of Developmental Neurobiology
and Neurophysiology, Institute of Neurobiology. Universidad
Nacional Autónoma de México. Campus UNAM Juriquilla, Querétaro, México.
This
article can be found at: https://eneurobiologia.uv.mx/index.php/eneurobiologia/article/view/2630
*Correspondence:
Department of Developmental Neurobiology and Neurophysiology. Institute of Neurobiology, Universidad Nacional Autónoma de
México. Campus UNAM Juriquilla, Querétaro, México.
E-mail
address: masal@unam.mx
DOI: https://doi.org/10.25009/eb.v15i37.2630
Recibido:
20 abril, 2024 | Aceptado: 20 mayo, 2024
Abstract
Studies in lactating rats show that
early food restriction induces deficient long-term cognitive states including
newborn care, motivation, and disturbed multimodal sensory information,
allowing dams to make decisions that enable pup survival. This study found a
correlation between certain cognitive responses of early underfed dams and
damages on the lateral habenula nucleus (LHN), which underlies motivational
responses. We investigated this relationship by restricting food from
gestational days (G) 6 to 12 (50%) and G13 to 19 (30%) and providing a balanced
diet from G20 to G21. After birth, pups were underfed by rotating two lactating
dams, one with tied nipples, every 12 hours. Pups were weaned at 25 days of age
and given an ad libitum diet. The F1 dams’ motivation (nest ratings,
retrieval latency, and anogenital licking) was assessed during 10 min daily on
lactating days (LDs) 4 and 12 when they were 90 days old. After the assessment
on LD12, dams were sacrificed, and their brains were processed (Golgi-Cox) to
measure the dendritic arbors and perikaryon of multipolar neurons from LHN.
Early underfed dams showed significant reductions in nest ratings and
anogenital licking and prolonged pup-retrieval latencies, concurrent with
significant decreases in the number and density of the dendritic arbors, as
well as in the cross-sectional area and perikaryon perimeter. The hypotrophy of
multipolar LHN neurons points to possible alterations in the excitability,
encoding, and integration of brain-descending code signals that modulate
maternal motivation.
Keywords: Perinatal undernutrition, maternal
motivation, lateral habenula, rats.
Resumen
En la rata, la restricción perinatal de alimento provoca
deficiencias cognitivas que alteran el cuidado maternal, la motivación y el
ingreso sensorial para la crianza. En el estudio, se correlacionan respuestas
de madres con desnutrición temprana, y el daño neuronal de la habénula lateral
que regula estados motivacionales. Se utilizaron hembras gestantes con bajos
porcentajes de alimento del día gestacional G) 6 al 12 (50%) y del G13 al 19
(30%), seguido de dieta balanceada del G20 al G21. Después del parto, las crías
continuaron su desnutrición rotando cada 12 h a dos madres lactantes, una con
pezones ligados. Destete en el día 25 de edad, seguido de dieta ad libitum.
La motivación materna (construcción del nido, latencia de acarreo, y lamido
anogenital) se evaluó (10 min), en los días 4 y 12 de lactancia en el día 90 de
edad. En el día 12 de lactancia, después de la prueba de motivación, las madres
se sacrificaron y se procesó su cerebro (Golgi-Cox) para evaluar dendritas y
somas de neuronas multipolares de la habénula lateral. Las madres que fueron
desnutridas redujeron significativamente la construcción del nido y el lamido
anogenital, incrementando su latencia para el acarreo de crías. Estos cambios
concurrieron con reducciones en el número y densidad de dendritas, área y
perímetro neuronal. La hipotrofia neuronal en la habénula lateral sugiere
alteraciones en la excitabilidad, codificación, e integración de señales
descendentes que modulan la motivación materna.
Palabras clave: Desnutrición
perinatal, motivación maternal, habénula lateral, ratas.
1. Introduction
Newborn rats give
their nursing dams multimodal sensory cues such as frequent ventral cutaneous
stimulation, to improve the efficiency of adaptive motor paradigms, and elicit
internal motivational and emotional states. These states are highly expressed
during the early stages of lactation and gradually decline near weaning.1
In turn, the dams exhibit intense fur stimulation, body licking, whisking
movements, retrieval, and vestibular activation during huddling and suckling,
all essential for the pups ‘physical and cognitive development.2-4
Perinatal
food restriction in the rat elicits long-term deficiencies in maternal care,
such as decreased nursing time, pup retrieval, and body licking, as well as
increased non-maternal behaviors like exaggerated self-grooming which
diminishes physical contact with the young. These alterations in maternal care
result in adaptive deficiencies when the pups enter adulthood, including
vulnerability to affective and motivational disorders.5-7
Early undernutrition also interferes with the progeny´s neuronal development in
several brain structures underlying cognitive responsiveness, as it reduces
cell numbers, dendritic branching, and the formation of spines with small
perikaryal alterations that restrict their connectivity and neuronal
interactions at cortical and subcortical levels.8-11
Neuroanatomical
and neurophysiological studies in rats have shown that the habenular nuclei in
both the medial and lateral subdivisions, maintain wide interconnections with
the neocortical layers, basal ganglia, and limbic, brainstem, and spinal motor
neurons to modulate the cognitive aspects of pups retrieval, nest building and
the nursing posture over pups.10,12-17 Furthermore,
focal bilateral electrolytic medial and lateral habenula lesions disrupted the
expression of motivation, emotional and cognitive responses, and maternal
responses for at least seven days.7,18,19
By contrast is unknown the diffuse damage linked to perinatal undernutrition on
lateral habenular nucleus (LHN) development and the motivation of lactating
rats. Therefore, we hypothesize that the damage to multipolar neurons in this
structure will interfere with the expression of some maternal components during
two stages of motivation during lactation.
2.
Materials
and Methods
2.1 Subjects
Animals
(n=20) were 90-day-old female Wistar rats (Rattus norvegicus,)
descendants of a stock originally obtained from Harlan Sprague-Dawley, Inc.,
and raised in an animal colony at the Laboratory Animal Facility of the
Institute of Neurobiology, National Autonomous University of Mexico (UNAM). The
Local Animal Committee approved animal care and protocols. They were conducted
under the guidelines for the care and use of mammals in Neuroscience and
Behavioral Research.20 Furthermore, animal
care and protocols were approved by Local Animal Committees project 108.A and
Official Mexican Standard NOM-062-Z00-199. Subjects were obtained from eight
pregnant, nulliparous dams at 100-120 days of age (200-250 g). All animals were
maintained in an automatically controlled room at 23 ± 2 ºC, 50% humidity on a
12-h light/dark cycle (lights on at 07:00 h), with ad libitum food
(Purina chow) and water. For mating, groups of four virgin female rats were
housed with two males of similar ages. Sperm-positive females were placed one
week before delivery in plastic maternity cages (35 x 27 x 17 cm3)
with grill tops, and woodchip bedding. The day of birth was referred to as
postnatal day 0. Twenty-four hours later, pups were weighed and sexed, and four
females and four males from each litter were randomly distributed among dams to
reduce possible genetic and nutritional differences that might influence the
experimental results. The presence of bilateral
thoracic and abdominal lines of nipples, as well as a shorter anogenital
distance in females were used as criteria for newborn sex recognition.
2.2 Nutritional Procedures
2.2.1. Control group (CG)
The
CG (n=10) was formed by lactating F1 dams obtained from four litters normally nourished
by well-fed mothers (G0) with free access to food and water during the
gestation and lactation periods. After birth, pups were fed and handled by
interchanging a pair of normally lactating mothers every 12 h for 24 days, as
described elsewhere.21 The female F1 dams
obtained through this procedure were mated and subjected to maternal testing at
90 days of age.
2.2.2. Underfed group (UG)
The
UG0 dams (n=10) came from four diffe-rent litters.
The standard food requirement was calculated by measuring food intake in four
pregnant rats (200-250 g) every week for 24 days. Each week's resulting average
food intake was the basal level used to calculate the food-intake percentage of
UG females. Dams were fed from gestational day 6 (G6) to G12 with 50% (9.5 g)
of the balanced diet (Purina chow), from G13 to G19 with 70% (13.3 g), and then
with 100% (19 g) of the same diet from G2 until parturition to avoid fetal
resorption or cannibalism of newborns. This protocol was chosen because
neurogenesis of the cortical and subcortical maternal circuit and afferent
connectivity occurs between G12 and G21.22
After birth, prenatally underfed F1 female newborns were nursed by two
lactating control dams. The main galactophorous ducts
were subcutaneously tied in one of these dams. The two lactating mothers were
interchanged every 12 h between litters from PDs 1 to 24. The CG and UG (F1)
groups of dams were weaned at PD25, after which rats were given free access to
water and food (Purina chow) (Figure 1). The F1 females were maintained in
groups of 4-6 until reaching 90 days of age when they were tested for maternal
behavior with their li-tters.21 This cross-fostering
procedure minimizes the effects of stress and maternal sensory deprivation on
the pups. Approximately 80% of UG dams were undernourished du-ring the light
phase of the cycle. This study evaluates the effects of pre- and neonatal
underfeeding paradigms on body and brain weights. Moreover, nest ratings,
retrieval latency, and anogenital licking were used to measure the lactating
dams´ motivational level. Additionally, in CG and UG F1 lactating dams we
evaluated the dendritic density and dendritic orders of branches, as well as
the cross-sectional area, and perikaryal perimeter of large multipolar LHN
neurons at LD12. The neuronal parameters were correlated with the long-term
cognitive performance of the mothers on lactating days (LDs) 4 and 12 when
their motivational response is at its highest or lowest values respectively.23,24
The
maternal response components were videotaped (10 min) in a sound-proof room
under continuous dim illumination provided by a red lamp (100 W=130 cd, Philips
Co., Amsterdam, Netherlands).
Figure
1. A) Underfeed procedure. B) Behavioral testing (nest ratings, retrieving
latency, and anogenital licking) for (n=10) CG and UG dams at LDs 4 and 12. C)
Morphometric evaluation of large multipolar LHN neurons in CG and UG at LD12 of
dams.
2.3 Histology
The
lactating dams were subjected to two dietary treatments (n=10, CG, and UG).
After the maternal evaluation, body and brain weights were recorded at PD90,
and LD12, to evaluate brain and behavioral development respectively.
Afterwards, the dams were deeply anesthetized with ether and transcardially perfused with saline and then with buffered
4% paraformaldehyde (JT Baker, Co.), pH 7.4. Next, the dams were decapitated,
and the brains were removed, weighted wet, cut into three coronal blocks, and
immersed in Golgi-Cox solution for impregnation. Three weeks later, the blocks
were dehydrated and embedded in low-viscosity nitrocellulose. Subsequently,
they were cut into 120-150 µm coronal sections and serially mounted. The slides
were coded to ensure blind evaluation concerning the dams age and dietary
treatment. When digitizing neuronal images, the experimenter only had access to
the code numbers, not the ages and nutritional treatments. The Paxinos and Watson atlas were used to identify and locate
the LHN.25 The anterior-posterior coordinates for
the location of the LHN corresponded to values ranging from Bregma -2.12 to
-3.80 mm. For each experimental group, 30 scattered and large multipolar LHN
neurons were analyzed, broken up by fiber bundles of the stria medullaris.
2.4 Morphometric
evaluation
After LD12, 60 well-impregnated multipolar LHN neurons
per group (n = 30) with dendritic arbors confined to one section were evaluated
for each experimental condition, age group, and neuronal parameter (Figure 2).
Dendritic density was measured by placing the cell body and primary dendrites
at the center of the first of a series of seven concentric rings (spaced at 40
µm intervals) and counting all dendritic intersections with larger individual
rings. Dendritic order measurements were obtained by counting the number of 1sts,
2nd, 3rd, 4th, and 5th dendritic
orders. Dendritic branches leaving the cell body were defined as the first
order, while those that branched from the former were considered second order,
and so on. Additionally, the cross-sectional area and perikaryal perimeter of
multipolar neurons were measured. In all cases neuronal measurements were
obtained at a magnification of 40 X using an image digitization system (Perception
Analysis System by Human-Computer Interface, Cambridge, UK). No attempt was
made to correct for compression of the three-dimensional dendritic arbor to a
two-dimensional sketch since the relative differences between neurons remain
constant when transformed from three to two dimensions. Furthermore, no
stereological method was used because the dendritic arbor is confined to the
tissue section. Additionally, the image analyzer performed some of the
previously described calculations for the soma parameters.
2.5 Statistics
All measurements were analyzed with ANOVA comparisons
(software Statistica 7): 1) Scores for
body and brain weight of CG vs.UG dams were submitted to a normality and
homogeneity of variance tests expressed as mean ± SD. Group measurements were
analyzed with a one-way ANOVA. 2) For the maternal nest ratings, retrieval
latency, and anogenital licking repeated measurements, a two-way ANOVA was
used, 2 (dietary regimes) X 2 (ages). 3) The effects of undernutrition on the
dendritic order and density of basilar branches of multipolar LHN neurons were
analyzed using a two-way ANOVA, 2 (dietary conditions) X 5 (dendritic orders)
or 7 (concentric rings). 4) The cross-sectional area and perimeter of
perikaryal measurements from CG and UG rats were compared with a one-way ANOVA.
The post hoc statistical comparisons between experimental groups were
performed using the Fisher’s (LSD) post hoc test. The alpha level for
all comparisons was set at p< 0.05.
3. Results
3.1. Effects on
body and brain weights of dams
Perinatal undernutrition affects the body and brain
weights of lactating dams.
According to
the ANOVA comparisons body and brain weight scores between CG and UG dams
significantly decreased due to perinatal undernutrition (Table 1).
Table 1.
Mean ± SEM of body and brain weights of GC and UG dams
(n = 10 /group).
Age (days) |
Body weight |
|
Brain weight (LD12) |
||||
CG |
UG |
|
CG |
UG |
|||
90 |
290.00 ±
11.72 |
258.80 ± 4.52 |
|
1.92 ±
0.037 |
1.70 ± 0.31 |
||
Factor |
df |
F |
p< |
|
F |
p< |
|
Diet |
1,8 |
6.16 |
0.037* |
|
20.16 |
0.002* |
|
3.2. Effects on
maternal responses
Perinatal undernutrition disrupted the early and late
motivational responses of dams.
3.2.1.
Nest ratings of lactating dams
The ANOVA
comparisons between groups indicated that nest ratings of UG F1 dams were
significantly reduced, F (1,14) = 38.95, p< 0.0001, with no effects
of age and interaction between factors. Post hoc comparisons indicated
that on LDs 4 and 12 UG dams showed significantly reduced nest ratings (p<0.05)
when compared with CG dams (Table 2).
3.2.2.
Pup- retrieval latency
The ANOVA comparisons
between groups showed prolonged significant retrieval latency exhibited by UG
F1 dams. F (1,14) = 9.45, p<0.05. There were no effects of age and
interaction between factors. Post hoc comparisons were only significant
on LD12 (Table 2).
3.2.3.
Maternal anogenital licking
The ANOVA
analysis showed significant reductions in the UG group, F (1,14) = 10.96, p<
0.005, without effects on age and no interaction between factors. Post hoc
comparisons were significantly reduced in the UG group on LDs 4 and 12 (Table
2).
Table 2.
Mean ± SEM of the maternal duration and latency of
components recorded in CG and UG dams at LDs 4 and 12 (n = 10/group). * p<0.05.
Behavior |
LD4 |
|
LD12 |
||
CG |
UG |
|
CG |
UG |
|
Nest ratings |
2.63 ± 0.18 |
1.50 ±
0.19* |
|
2.75 ± 0.16 |
1.88 ±
0.13* |
Retrieval latency |
386 ± 201 |
396 ± 295 |
|
284 ± 157 |
732 ± 198* |
Anogenital licking |
759 ± 60 |
369 ± 76* |
|
681 ± 60 |
532 ± 99* |
3.3. Morphometric
analyses
Perinatal undernutrition
disrupted the development of multipolar LHN neurons in lactating dams.
3.4. Effects on
the dendritic arbor
The density of
the dendritic circle crossings of multipolar LHN neurons of dams, measured as
the number of dendrites with seven circles, showed significant reductions
associated with the diet (F1,58=24.901, p<0.0006) and the number
of circles (7) (F6,348=291.174; p<0.0001) without interaction
between factors. Post hoc comparisons of dendritic crossings indicated
that the UG of dams were significantly reduced (p<0.05) from the 2nd-5th
number of circles on LD12 (Figure 2A).
The number of
the dendritic orders of multipolar LHN neurons of dams indicated significant
reductions associated with the diet (F1,58=20.97, p<0.0002) and
the dendritic orders (5) (F4,232=254.091, p<0.00001. Moreover, a
significant interaction between factors interaction (F4,232) =3.492,
p<0.008. Post hoc comparisons indicated that UG multipolar neurons
showed significant reductions (p<0.05) in the 3rd, 4th,
and the 5th dendritic orders on LD12 (Figure 2B).
3.5. Effects on
perikaryal measurements
The ANOVA
comparisons of the perikaryon area measurements showed significant reductions
in the multipolar LHN neurons of the UG dams (F1,58=543.97,
p<0.001) (Figure 2C). ANOVA comparisons of the soma perimeter measures
indicated significant reductions associated with diet (F1,58=422.36,
p<0.001).
Figure 2. Mean
values ± SEM at LD12 of A) Dendritic crossings, B) Dendritic orders, and C)
Perikaryal measurements of multipolar LHN neurons during the development of CG
and UG groups of dams. * p<0.05, ** p<0.001. Note the
general significant decrements in the dendritic crossings, number of dendritic
orders, and perikaryal values in the UG dams.
4. Discussion
The current findings showed that perinatal
undernutrition induced significant reductions in body and brain weights of dams
between G0 and LD25, and at 90 days of age, they were on a balanced diet and
tested for maternal behavior. The prenatal undernutrition established in F0
dams included 30% to 50% food restriction (G6 to G20), which possibly
interfered with the size and weight of the placenta and with fetal nutrition,
as described elsewhere.26,27 F1 pups from
PDs 1 to 24 were underfed by rotating two well-fed lactating rats one with tied
nipples between litters, resulting in significant sensory deficiencies in
mother-litter interactions, tactile stimulation, and long-term cognitive
outcomes.7,21 These
alterations are consistent with those in previous reports, suggesting that
several factors, such as the abnormal structure and function of the placenta,
and the reduced tactile sensitivity in altered mother-litter interactions, lead
to decreased physical contact and pup licking with long-term behavioral
consequences.2,4 These findings
also align with studies in isolate–reared rats, which were less attentive to
their young, as they performed fewer pup retrievals and spent less time licking
their pups.28 Moreover, early undernutrition disrupts the release
of various hormones and growth factors that promote proteins, body and cellular,
glia, and brain synaptic development that impact body weight, which constitutes
tangible evidence of the harmful effects of early food restriction.14,29-31 Additionally,
the present data showed that UG lactating dams exhibited significantly reduced
motivational responses that interfered with the nurturing of their pups, as
reflected by the decrease in nest ratings, anogenital licking on LDs 4 and 12,
and retrieval latency on LD12. The fin-dings of this study also indicated that
both pre- and neonatal food restriction in UG dams resulted in consistent
reductions in dendritic branch density and number of dendrites, from medial to
the distal portions for the synaptic reception of the multipolar LHN area,
compared with the CG of dams on LD12. Although we did not evaluate the number
and distribution of afferent dendritic frameworks, previous reports have shown
that the large diencephalic multipolar LHN substrate generated between G13 and
G16 remains a well-conserved epithalamic structure that receives inputs from
the medial prefrontal cortex, olfactory bulb, basal ganglia, and la-teral hypothalamus between G14 and G16. This is generally
concurrent with the rat´s cognitive motivational appearance.32,33 LHN efferent
projections reach the midbrain, brainstem, and spinal cord, modulating motor
actions, stress, mood, and motivational decisions.16,34 The
motivational deficiencies of UG dams in this study could be partly associated
with the development of LHN neurons and with the LHN hypotrophy caused by
perinatal undernutrition. These deficiencies interfere with their modulatory
influence upon the lower brainstem, disrupting the release of dopamine and
serotonin neurons and the appearance of maternal-altered motor and cognitive
responses for pups survival as described elsewhere.32,35 Furthermore,
motivational alterations in UG dams may be related to the pups´ delayed sensory
development and the restricted environmental stimulation, both of which
hindered the anatomical and functional development of the young.2,4,36,37
On G15,
amniotic fluid in the rat uterus is constantly modified within a narrow range
of conditions that the fetus needs to challenge in addition to the
somatosensory and chemosensory cues that impact its brain development for a
less stable environmental habitat. Thus, the fetus mainly responds to the
activation of chemical signals that reach the brain through the placenta and
travel through the immature embryonic olfactory and gustatory systems. These
sensory systems advance in their development by birth and undergo rapid
maturation. They also have increased plasticity to promote cognitive processes
such as maternal odor and taste recognition, early learning, attentiveness, and
motivation outcomes.38-41 Several
studies indicate that somatosensory in the rat input appears early during
gestation, in contrast to the mainly postpartum origins of ear and eye-opening.42 The role of
this early tactile sensory system in prenatal cognitive development is still
unspecified. Furthermore, early undernutrition significantly delays and
restricts these sensory channels' anatomical and functional development.7,9 However, how
these sensory influences contribute to attention, mood, and motivational
development later in life is still poorly understood.2,15,43
In the rat, the
olfactory bulb macro neurons (mitral and tufted cells) are formed between days
G12 and G15. They gradually proliferate and increase their connectivity in the
following prenatal days, achieving their functional maturity at birth.41 However, their
growth and morphological organization in layers occur after birth. The total
physiological activity of macro neurons is achieved perinatally when wide
interconnections with granule cells and precise interconnections within the
olfactory glomeruli are esta-blished.44 The
neurogenetic processes of macro neurons continue in the olfactory bulb during
the first three weeks of life, whereas interneurons undergo these processes
even in adulthood.45 Although olfactory bulb macro neurons are formed
before birth, perinatal undernourishment at the LHN level may have long-lasting
functional consequences on the maternal underfed motivational responses of underfed
dams. These findings suggest that UG dams exhibit alterations relevant to
newborn discrimination since olfaction is a fundamental sensory avenue for
early adaptive responses such as precocious learning, motivation, and maternal
care of the progeny.44 Our findings showed long-term
motivational deficiencies in early underfed lactating dams. These deficiencies
are concurrent with not previously described LHN neuronal damages, possibly
deteriorating the efferent cortico-subcortical signals underlying maternal
developmental cognition. However, further studies are needed to understand how
early food restriction and the associated delayed sensory development, may
impact the morphological and functional brain substrate implicated in cognitive
phenomena by using electrophysiological, anatomical, cellular, and molecular
techniques.
5. Conclusion
Our findings
showed that the perinatal underfeeding paradigm in this study which evaluates
high or low maternal motivational responses, was associated with the hypotrophy
of multipolar LHN neurons of UG dams. These neurons possibly relayed different
code information from cortical and subcortical structures to modulate deficient
maternal cognitive responses. The findings provide evidence that disrupted
mother-newborn interactions due to perinatal food restriction may be a robust
source of brain damage associated with potential psychiatric disorders, working
memory impairment, decision-making, drug addiction, and altered affective
behaviors. These anatomical and functional disorders of the brain, observed in
the early underfed model of lactating dams, may be helpful to understanding the
cognitive disarrays relating to perinatal nursing commonly observed in humans
in underdeveloped countries. However, further studies using the current model
may be challenged and needed by the exposure to different dietary paradigms,
reaction to various novel environmental cues, under-brain disorders or
associated with addictive drug exposure. Further studies are necessary to
understand how the exposure in early life to food restriction adversity
increases the vulnerability to several disorders including affective and reward dysfunctions.46
6. Acknowledgments
This work was partly supported by DGAPA/UNAM,
IN200317. We thank Jessica González Norris for the editorial assistance and
helpful suggestions, and V. Avalos for collecting data.
7. Conflict of interest
No conflict of interest was reported by the authors
regarding this publication.
8. References
1. Wansaw
MP, Pereira M, Morrell JI. Characterization of maternal motivation in the
lactating rat: contrasts between early and late postpartum responses. Horm Behav. 2008 54: 294-301.
2. Ardiel EI, Rankin CH. The importance of
touch in development. Paediatr Child Health. 2010 15:
153-156.
3. Broutte-Lahlou I, Vernet-Maury E, Vigouroux M. Role of pups´ ultrasonic calls in a particular
maternal behavior in Wistar rat: pups ‘anogenital licking. Behav
Brain Res. 50: 1992 147-154.
4. Soriano O, Regalado M, Torrero C, Salas
M. Contributions of undernutrition and handling to huddling development of
rats. Physiol Behav. 2006
89: 543-551.
5. Andersen SL. Stress, sensitive periods, and substance abuse. Neurobiol of Stress. 2019 10: 100140.
6. Salas M, Regalado M, Torrero C. Recovery
of long-term maternal behavioral deficiencies of neonatally underfed rats by
early sensory stimulation: effects of successive parturitions. Nutr Neurosci. 2001 4: 311-322.
7. Salas M, Ortiz-Valladares M, Torrero C,
Regalado M. Kyphotic response alterations in perinatally
underfed lactating dams. eNeurobiol. 2022a 13:
111222.
8. Macri S, Würbel
H. Developmental plasticity of HPA and fear responses in rats: a critical
review of the maternal mediation hypothesis. Horm Behav. 2006 50: 667-680.
9. Ortiz-Valladares M, Torrero C, Regalado
M, Salas M. Late-emerging effects of perinatal undernutrition in neuronal
limbic structures underlying the maternal response in the rat. Brain Res. 2018
1700: 31-40.
10. Salas M, Torrero C, Regalado M, Rubio L.
Effects of perinatal undernutrition on the basilar dendritic arbor of the
anterior cingulate pyramidal neurons in lactating dams. Acta Neurobiol Exp. 2015 75: 372-380.
11. Torrero C, Regalado M, Rubio L, Salas M.
Effects of neonatal undernutrition on development of the dorsolateral
prefrontal cortex pyramidal cells in the rat. J Behav
Brain Sci. 2014 4: 49-57.
12. Afonso VM, Sison M, Lovic
V, Fleming AS. Medial prefrontal cortex in the female rat affect sexual and
maternal behavior and their sequential organization. Behav
Neurosci. 2007 121: 515-526.
13. Christoph GR, Leonzio RJ, Wilcox KS.
Stimulation of the lateral habenula inhibits dopamine-containing neurons in the
substantia nigra and ventral tegmental area of the rat. J Neurosci.
1986 6: 613-619.
14. Lecourtier L, Kelly PH. Bilateral
lesions of the habenula induce attentional disturbances in rats.
Neuropsychopharmacology. 2005 30: 484-496.
15. Salas M, Torrero C, Regalado M. Effects
of pre- and neonatal undernutrition on long-term hearing cognition of the rat.
J Behav Brain Sci. 2022b 12: 302-322.
16. Stopper CM, Florescano SB. What´s better
for me? Fundamental role for lateral habenula in promoting subjective decision
biases. Nature Neurosci. 2013 17: 33-35.
17. Sutherland RJ. The dorsal diencephalic conduction system: a review
of the anatomy and functions of the habenular complex. Neurosci
Biobehav Rev. 1982 6: 1-13.
18. Corodimas KP, Rosenblatt JS, Canfield ME, Morrell JI.
Neurons in the lateral subdivision of the habenular complex mediate the normal
onset of maternal behavior in rats. Behav Neurosci. 1993 5: 827-843.
19. Numan, M. Motivational systems, and the neural circuitry of
maternal behavior in the rat. Dev Psychobiol. 2007
49: 12-21.
20. National Research Council. Nutrient requirements of laboratory
animals. Fourth Revised Edition. The National Academies Press, Washington, DC.
1995.
21. Felix J, Regalado M, Torrero C, Salas M.
Retrieval of pups by female rats undernourished during the pre- and neonatal
period. J Behav Brain Sci. 2014 4: 325-333.
22. Altman J, Bayer SA. Atlas of prenatal rat brain development. CRC
Press, Boca Raton, Florida 1995 pp. 1-589.
23. Reisbick S, Rosenblatt JS, Mayer AD.
Decline of Maternal Behavior in the virgin and lactating rat. J Comp Physiol Psychol. 1975 89: 722-732.
24. Yamamuro Y, Sensui
N. Maternal behavior, and emotional status of mother rats at different stages
of lactation. Anim Sci J. 2000 71: 208-213.
25. Paxinos G, Watson CH. The rat brain in stereotaxic
coordinates. Sixth edition, Academic Press, Cambridge, USA, 2006.
26. Belkacemi L, Nelson DM, Desai M, Ross
MG. Maternal undernutrition influences placental-fetal development. Biol Reprod. 2010 83: 325-331.
27. Jansson N, Pettersson J, Haafiz A, Ericsson A, Palmberg I,
Tranberg M, Ganapathy V, Powel TL, Jansson T.
Down-regulation of placental transport of amino acids precedes the development
of intrauterine growth restriction in rats fed a low protein diet. J Physiol.
2006 576: 935-946.
28. Gonzalez A, Lovic V, Ward GR, Wainwright
PE, Fleming AS. Intergenerational effects of complete maternal deprivation and
replacement stimulation on maternal behavior and emotionality in female rats. Dev Psychobiol. 2001 38: 11-32.
29. Aláez
C, Calvo R, Obregón MJ, Pascual-Leone AM. Thyroid hormones and 5’-deiodinase
activity in neonatal undernourished rats. Endocrinology. 1992 130: 773–779.
30. Ketelslegers JM, Maiter
D, Maes M, Underwood LE. Thissen
JP. Nutritional regulation of the growth hormone and insulin-like growth
factor-binding proteins. Horm Res. 1996 45: 252–257.
31. Schanberg SM, Evoniuk
G, Kuhn CM. Tactile and nutritional aspects of maternal care specific
regulators of neuroendocrine function and cellular development. Proc Soc Exp
Biol Med. 1984 175: 135–146.
32. Ables JL, Park K, Ibañez-Tallon I.
Understanding the habenula: A major node in circuits regulating emotion and
motivation. Pharm Res. 2023 190: 106734.
33. Altman J, Bayer SA. Development of the diencephalon in the rat.
IV. Quantitative study of the time of origin of neurons and the internuclear
chronological gradients in the thalamus. J Comp Neurol. 1979 188: 455-471.
34. Geisler S, Trimble M. The lateral habenula: no longer neglected.
CNS Spectrums. 2008 13: 484-489.
35. Matsumoto M, Hikosaka O. Lateral
habenula as a source of negative reward signals in dopamine neurons. Nature.
2007 447: 1111-1115.
36. Lopez-Jimenez D, Torrero C, Regalado M,
Salas, M. Effects of perinatal undernutrition and massage stimulation upon the
ambiguous nucleus in the rat prior to weaning. J Behav
Brain Sci. 2013 3: 200-209.
37. Tonkiss J, Bonnie KE, Hudson JL, Shultz
PL, Duran P, Galler JR. Ultrasonic call
characteristics of rat pups are altered following prenatal malnutrition. Dev Psychobiol. 2003 43: 90-101.
38. Brunjes PC, Frazier LL. Maturation and
plasticity in the olfactory system of vertebrates. Brain Res Rev. 1986 11:
1-45.
39. Hall WG, Bryan TE. The ontogeny of feeding in rats: IV. Taste
development as measured by intake and behavioral responses to oral infusion of
sucrose and quinine. J Comp Physiol Psychol. 1981 95:
240-251.
40. Moriceau S, Sullivan
RM. Maternal presence serves as a switch between learning fear and attraction
in infancy. Nat Neurosci. 2006 9: 1004-1006.
41. Salas M, Schapiro S. Behavioral responses of
infant rats to maternal odor. Physiol Behav. 1970 5: 815-817
42. Alberts JR, Cramer CP. Ecology and Experience.
In: Blass EM, Ed., Developmental Psychobiology and Behavioral Ecology, Handbook
of Behavioral Neurobiology, 9, Springer, Boston, 1988 pp.1-39.
43. Panksepp J, Biven L.
Ancestral passions. In: Panksepp, J. and Biven L.,
Eds., The archaeology of mind, WW. Norton & Company, New York, Chapter 2012
8, 283-310.
44. Frias C, Torrero C,
Regalado M, Salas M. Development of mitral cells and olfactory bulb layers in
neonatally undernourished rats. Nutr Neurosci. 2009 12: 97-104
45. Rosselli-Austin L, Altman J. The postnatal development of
the main olfactory bulb of the rat. J Dev Physiol. 1979 1: 295–313.
46.
Salcedo C, Torrero C, Regalado M, Rubio L, Salas M.
Effects of pre- and neonatal undernutrition on the kyphotic response and
c-Fos’s activity in the caudal periaqueductal gray of primiparous lactating
Wistar rats. Physiol Behav.
2018 185: 8 7–94.
Enlaces refback
- No hay ningún enlace refback.
eNeurobiología es una publicación cuatrimestral editada por el Instituto de Investigaciones Cerebrales de la Universidad Veracruzana. Estamos ubicados en Av. Dr. Luis Castelazo Ayala, s/n, colonia Industrial Ánimas, C.P. 91190, Xalapa-Enríquez, Veracruz, México. Teléfono: 8418900 ext. 13062, www.iice.uv.mx; eneurobiologia@uv.mx. Reserva de Derechos al Uso Exclusivo 04-2023-061314100600-102, otorgada por el Instituto Nacional de Derechos de Autor. ISSN: 2007-3054. Esta obra está bajo una Licencia Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International.